reading
Lige nu læser du disse ord uden at tænke ret meget over det, eller uden at yde en bevidst anstrengelse. I lynhurtige ryk bevæger dine øjne sig hen over skærmen fra venstre til højre, mens du undervejs skaber mening ud af det, som ellers blot ville være en serie af sorte tegn.

Læsning er for dig ikke let - det er automatisk. At se på et ord uden at læse det er næsten umuligt, fordi tandhjulene i behandling af skrevet sprog igangsættes, så snart en øvet læser ser tekst.

Og dog, hvor fristende det end kan være at tænke, at læsning er naturligt indlagt i hjernen, så bliv ikke snydt. Læsning er ikke let. Det er ikke engang naturligt. .

De første eksempler på skriftsprog går tilbage til ca 5000 år siden, som er lille brøkdel af de 60.000 år eller mere, hvor mennesker har anvendt talte sprog.

Det betyder, at vores art ikke har haft nok tid til at udvikle et netværk i hjernen, som giver os forudsætninger for at lære læsning. det er kun gennem års øvning og undervisning, at vi har vi selv har smedet disse forbindelser.


Kommentar: Delvist oversat af Sott.net fra How learning to read changes your brain


How the brain learns to read

Brains are constantly reorganising themselves. Any time we learn a new skill, connections between neurons that allow us to perform that skill become stronger. This flexibility is heightened during childhood, which is why so much learning gets crammed in before adolescence.

As a child becomes literate, there is no "reading centre" that magically materialises in the brain. Instead, a network of connections develops to link existing areas that weren't previously linked. Reading becomes a way of accessing language by sight, which means it builds on architecture that is already used for recognising visual patterns and understanding spoken language.

The journey of a word

When a skilled reader encounters a printed word, that information travels from their eyes to their occipital lobe (at the back of the brain), where it is processed like any other visual stimulus.

From there, it travels to the left fusiform gyrus, otherwise known as the brain's "letterbox". This is where the black squiggles are recognised as letters in a word. The letterbox is a special stopover on the word's journey because it only develops as the result of learning to read. It doesn't exist in very young children or illiterate adults, and it's activated less in people with dyslexia, who have a biological difference in the way their brains process written text.

Words and letters are stored in the letterbox - not as individually memorised shapes or patterns, but as symbols. This is why a skilled reader can recognise a word quickly, regardless of font, cAsE, or typeface.

Information then travels from the letterbox to the frontal and temporal lobes of the brain, to work out word meaning and pronunciation. These same areas are activated when we hear a word, so they are specialised for language, rather than just reading and writing.

Because information can travel so quickly across the skilled reader's synaptic highways, the entire journey takes less than half a second.

But what happens in the brain of a five-year-old child, whose highways are still under construction?

The growing brain

For young children, the process of getting from print to meaning is slow and effortful. This is partly because beginning readers have not yet built up a store of familiar words that they can recognise by sight, so they must instead "sound out" each letter or letter sequence.

Every time children practise decoding words, they forge new connections between the visual and spoken language areas of the brain, gradually adding new letters and words to the brain's all-important letterbox.

Remember, when a practised reader recognises a word by sight, they process the letters in that word, rather than its shape.

Literacy instruction can therefore support children's learning by highlighting the symbolic nature of letters - in other words, by drawing attention to the relationships between letters and speech sounds.

Here, evidence from brain imaging research and educational research converge to show that early phonics instruction can help construct an efficient reading network in the brain.

What might the future hold for literacy development?

As technology evolves, so too must our definition of what it means to be "literate". Young brains now need to adapt not only to written language, but also to the fast-paced media through which written language is presented.

Only time will tell how this affects the development of that mysterious beige sponge between our ears.